To use all functions of this page, please activate cookies in your browser.
my.bionity.com
With an accout for my.bionity.com you can always see everything at a glance – and you can configure your own website and individual newsletter.
- My watch list
- My saved searches
- My saved topics
- My newsletter
Müllerian mimicry
Additional recommended knowledgeMüllerian mimicry (ü pronounced yu) is a natural phenomenon where two or more harmful species that are not closely related, and share one or more common predators, have come to mimic each other's warning signals. It is named after the German naturalist Fritz Müller, who first proposed the concept in 1878.[2][3] In Batesian mimicry a harmless organism imitating the protected species is referred to as the mimic and the dangerous one being imitated the model. Müllerian mimicry presents a problem because both parties are harmful; each mimic the other species, while serving as a model at the same time. If one species is encountered far less the other, the more common species could be treated as the model and the other the mimic.[4] However, if they are encountered in similar numbers they would best be termed comimics or mimic-models. The predator mediating indirect convergence between these two parties is known as the signal receiver or dupe, though the latter term is less relevant here, as they are not actually deceived about the harmful qualities of their prey; both prey provide an honest warning signal. For this reason, some have suggested it is not a form of mimicry at all, as no deception is involved. Unlike other mimicry systems, the signal receiver is better off for mistaking one harmful species for another, as it avoids the potential harm involved. However, because the mimic may have a degree of protection itself, the distinction is not absolute, and there can be said to be a spectrum between the two forms.[5] Additionally, a species may be a Batesian mimic to one predator and a Müllerian mimic to another. While Batesian and Müllerian mimicry are commonly given examples of mimicry, there is often little or no mention of other forms.[6] There are many other types of mimicry however, some very similar in principle, others far separated. For example in aggressive mimicry a predator might mimic the food of its prey, luring them towards it and improving its foraging capabilities. Müllerian mimicry need not involve visual mimicry; it may employ involve any of the senses. For example, many snakes share the same auditory warning signals, an auditory Müllerian mimicry ring. More than one common signal may show convergences by the parties. While model and mimic are often closely related species, Müllerian mimicry between very distantly related taxa also occurs. BackgroundMüllerian mimicry was proposed by the German zoologist and naturalist Johann Friedrich Theodor Müller (1821-1897), always known as Fritz. An early proponent of evolution, Müller offered the first explanation for resemblance between certain butterflies that had puzzled the English naturalist Henry Walter Bates, who, like Müller, spent a significant part of his life in Brazil. Müller had also seen these butterflies first hand, and collected specimens like Bates. Understanding Müllerian mimicry is impossible without first understanding aposematism, or warning signals. Dangerous organisms with these aposematic signals are able to avoid predators, who quickly learn after a bad experience not to pursue the same prey again. Learning is not actually necessary for animals which instinctively avoid certain prey,[7] however learning from experience is much more common.[8] The underlying concept with predators that learn is that the warning signal makes the harmful organism easier to remember than if it remained as cryptic as possible (e.g. being still and silent, providing no scent, and blending in with the background). Aposematism and crypsis are in this way opposing concepts, but this does not mean they are mutually exclusive. Many animals remain inconspicuous until threatened, then suddenly employ warning signals, such as bright colors on their underside or loud vocalizations. In this way, they enjoy the best of both strategies. Many different prey of the same predator may employ separate warning colors, but this makes no sense for any party. Surely if they could all get together and agree on a common warning signal the predator would have less detrimental experiences, and the prey would lose less individuals educating it. But no such conference needs to take place, as a prey species that just so happens to look a little like a harmful species will be safer than its conspecifics, leading to a tendency toward a single warning language. This can lead to the evolution of both Batesian and Müllerian mimicry, depending on whether the prey is harmful as well or just a freerider. Multiple species can join this protective cooperative, expanding the mimicry ring. Müller thus provided an explanation for 'Bate's paradox'; the mimicry was not a case of exploitation by one species but rather a mutualistic arrangement. Notes and references
Further reading
|
||||||||||
This article is licensed under the GNU Free Documentation License. It uses material from the Wikipedia article "Müllerian_mimicry". A list of authors is available in Wikipedia. |